Match!

Kids becoming less alike: A behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence

Published on Sep 1, 2019in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology5.919
· DOI :10.1037/pspp0000194
René Mõttus24
Estimated H-index: 24
,
Daniel Andrew Briley16
Estimated H-index: 16
(University of Texas at Austin)
+ 5 AuthorsElliot M. Tucker-Drob31
Estimated H-index: 31
(University of Texas at Austin)
Sources
Abstract
Recent work in personality development has indicated that the magnitude of individual differences in personality increases over child development. Do such patterns reflect the differentiation of individuals by genotype, an increasing influence of environmental factors, or some (interactive) combination of the two? Using a population-based sample of over 2,500 twins and multiples from the Texas Twin Project, we estimated age trends in the variances in self- and parent-reported measures of the Big Five personality traits between Ages 8 and 18 years. We then estimated age trends in the genetic and environmental components of variance in each measure. Individual differences in personality increased in magnitude from childhood through mid-adolescence. This pattern emerged using both children’s self-reports and ratings provided by their parents, and was primarily attributable to increases in the magnitude of genetic influences. Most of the increasing genetic variance appeared nonadditive, pointing to the possibility that developmental processes tend to make genetically similar individuals disproportionately more alike in their personality traits over time. These findings could reflect increasing or accumulating effects of trait-by-trait interactions; person-by-environment transactions, whereby genetically similar people are disproportionally likely to experience similar environments; the activation of dominant genes across developmental transitions (e.g., puberty); or some combination of these three processes, among other factors. Theories of personality development will need to accommodate these descriptive findings, and longitudinal, genetically informed designs are needed to test some of the specific hypotheses springing from this study. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved)o
Figures & Tables
  • References (74)
  • Citations (1)
📖 Papers frequently viewed together
5 Citations
14 Citations
170 Citations
78% of Scinapse members use related papers. After signing in, all features are FREE.
References74
Newest
#1René MõttusH-Index: 24
#2Michael AllerhandH-Index: 1
7 Citations
#1Dorret I. Boomsma (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 136
#2Quinta Helmer (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 3
Last. Eco J. C. de Geus (VU: VU University Amsterdam)H-Index: 97
view all 15 authors...
For the participants in the Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) we constructed the extended pedigrees which specify all relations among nuclear and larger twin families in the register. A total of 253,015 subjects from 58,645 families were linked to each other, to the degree that we had information on the relations among participants. We describe the algorithm that was applied to construct the pedigrees. For > 30,000 adolescent and adult NTR participants data were available on harmonized neuroticism...
10 CitationsSource
#1René Mõttus (UT: University of Tartu)H-Index: 24
#2Christopher J. Soto (Colby College)H-Index: 20
Last. Helena R. Slobodskaya (NSU: Novosibirsk State University)H-Index: 20
view all 3 authors...
Do individual differences in personality traits become more or less pronounced over childhood and adolescence? The present research examined age differences in the variance of a range of personality traits, using parent reports of two large samples of children from predominantly the USA and Russia, respectively. Results indicate (i) that individual differences in most traits tend to increase with age from early childhood into early adolescence and then plateau, (ii) that this general pattern of ...
6 CitationsSource
#1David Bierbach (Leibniz Association)H-Index: 15
#2Kate L. Laskowski (Leibniz Association)H-Index: 12
Last. Max Wolf (Leibniz Association)H-Index: 18
view all 3 authors...
Genetically-identical animals experiencing the same environmental conditions should develop, in theory, identical behavioral traits. However, Bierbach et al. show here that behavioral differences still emerge among cloned fish under tightly controlled experimental conditions.
47 CitationsSource
#1René Mõttus (Edin.: University of Edinburgh)H-Index: 24
#2Christian KandlerH-Index: 24
Last. Robert R. McCraeH-Index: 111
view all 5 authors...
It has been argued that facets do not represent the bottom of the personality hierarchy—even more specific personality characteristics, nuances, could be useful for describing and understanding individuals and their differences. Combining 2 samples of German twins, we assessed the consensual validity (correlations across different observers), rank-order stability, and heritability of nuances. Personality nuances were operationalized as the 240 items of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-...
54 CitationsSource
#1Daniel Andrew Briley (University of Texas at Austin)H-Index: 16
#2Elliot M. Tucker-Drob (University of Texas at Austin)H-Index: 31
Empirical studies of cognitive ability and personality have tended to operate in isolation of one another. We suggest that returning to a unified approach to considering the development of individual differences in both cognition and personality can enrich our understanding of human development. We draw on previous meta-analyses of longitudinal, behavior genetic studies of cognition and personality across the life span, focusing particular attention on age trends in heritability and differential...
25 CitationsSource
#1Brent W. Roberts (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 68
#2Jing Luo (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 5
Last. Patrick L. Hill (Carleton University)H-Index: 27
view all 6 authors...
The current meta-analysis investigated the extent to which personality traits changed as a result of intervention, with the primary focus on clinical interventions. We identified 207 studies that had tracked changes in measures of personality traits during interventions, including true experiments and prepost change designs. Interventions were associated with marked changes in personality trait measures over an average time of 24 weeks (e.g., d = .37). Additional analyses showed that the increas...
121 CitationsSource
#1Min-Tzu LoH-Index: 12
#2David A. HindsH-Index: 55
Last. Chi-Hua ChenH-Index: 26
view all 22 authors...
Personality is influenced by genetic and environmental factors1 and associated with mental health. However, the underlying genetic determinants are largely unknown. We identified six genetic loci, including five novel loci2, 3, significantly associated with personality traits in a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies (N = 123,132–260,861). Of these genome-wide significant loci, extraversion was associated with variants in WSCD2 and near PCDH15, and neuroticism with variants on chromo...
107 CitationsSource
#1Brent W. Roberts (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 68
#2Lauren B. Nickel (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 4
As part of the resurgence of interest in personality development, we wrote several theoretical pieces outlining the Neo-Socioanalytic Model of Personality Development. In this chapter, we clarify why we developed this framework and how it differs from other personality frameworks. We also discuss how well components of the framework, such as the principles of personality development, have held up over time. We conclude with recommendations on improving the model and for future research to better...
15 CitationsSource
#1Christian Kandler (Bielefeld University)H-Index: 24
#2Michael Papendick (Bielefeld University)H-Index: 1
Abstract In this chapter, we introduce three perspectives of how quantitative behavior genetic modeling can broaden our knowledge about the etiology of personality differences, stability, and change. First, based on the data from 14 cross-sectional, 13 longitudinal, and 3 cross-sequential studies, we illustrate age trends of the genetic and environmental contributions to individual differences in five personality trait dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscient...
9 CitationsSource
Cited By1
Newest
#1Yayouk E. Willems (PHRI: Public Health Research Institute)H-Index: 3
#2Odilia M. Laceulle (UU: Utrecht University)H-Index: 9
Last. Catrin Finkenauer (UU: Utrecht University)H-Index: 36
view all 4 authors...
Family connectedness is key for the development of self-control in early and middle childhood. But is family connectedness still important during the transitional phase of adolescence, when adolescents demand more independence from their parents and rely more on their peers? The aim of the present study was to investigate the association between family connectedness and self-control, and whether it still holds in adolescence using a genetically sensitive design. Data were used from a large sampl...
Source
#1Daniel Andrew Briley (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 16
#2Jonathan M Livengood (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 9
Last. Brent W. Roberts (UIUC: University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign)H-Index: 68
view all 6 authors...
Behavior genetic findings figure in debates ranging from urgent public policy matters to perennial questions about the nature of human agency. Despite a common set of methodological tools, behavior genetic studies approach scientific questions with potentially divergent goals. Some studies may be interested in identifying a complete model of how individual differences come to be (e.g., identifying causal pathways among genotypes, environments, and phenotypes across development). Other studies pl...
3 CitationsSource