Match!

Differences in Shame and Pride as a Function of Children's Gender and Task Difficulty

Published on Jun 1, 1992in Child Development
· DOI :10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01651.x
Michael Lewis81
Estimated H-index: 81
(UMDNJ: University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey),
Steven M. Alessandri17
Estimated H-index: 17
(Drexel University),
Margaret Wolan Sullivan30
Estimated H-index: 30
(UMDNJ: University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey)
Abstract
3-year-old children were presented with easy and difficult tasks and their emotional responses of shame and pride were observed. No shame was shown when subjects succeeded on the tasks and no pride was shown when they failed. Significantly more shame was shown when subjects failed easy tasks than when they failed difficult tasks, and significantly more pride was shown when subjects succeeded on difficult than on easy tasks. While there were no sex differences in task failures, girls showed more shame than boys. There were no sex differences in pride when subjects succeeded.
  • References (35)
  • Citations (184)
📖 Papers frequently viewed together
1992
279 Citations
1971
1,440 Citations
408 Citations
78% of Scinapse members use related papers. After signing in, all features are FREE.
References35
Newest
#1Michael Lewis (RU: Rutgers University)H-Index: 81
Shame, the quintessential human emotion, received little attention during the years in which the central forces believed to be motivating us were identified as primitive instincts like sex and aggression. Now, redressing the balance, there is an explosion of interest in the self-conscious emotion. Much of our psychic lives involve the negotiation of shame, asserts Michael Lewis, internationally known developmental and clinical psychologist. Shame is normal, not pathological, though opposite reac...
667 Citations
: Individual differences in proneness to shame and proneness to guilt are thought to play an important role in the development of both adaptive and maladaptive interpersonal and intrapersonal processes. But little empirical research has addressed these issues, largely because no reliable, valid measure has been available to researchers interested in differentiating proneness to shame from proneness to guilt. The Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory (SCAAI) was developed to assess char...
396 CitationsSource
#1Klaus Schneider (RUB: Ruhr University Bochum)H-Index: 7
#2Karoline HanneH-Index: 1
Last. Beate LehmannH-Index: 1
view all 3 authors...
Abstract Children between the ages of 3 and 6 years were asked to predict their success or failure in two different tasks: a box-lifting task and a task in which marbles had to be rolled through gates of different widths. Both tasks had five difficulty levels which were presented either simultancously or successively. Along with the verbal or nonverbal responses to questions about their chances of success, the time children needed to make the prediction (decision time), the number of eye movemen...
12 CitationsSource
#1Michael Lewis (RU: Rutgers University)H-Index: 81
Last. M. WeissH-Index: 2
view all 4 authors...
In each of 2 studies, the mirror-rouge technique was used to differentiate children into those who showed self-recognition and those who did not. In Study 1, 27 children (aged 9-24 months) were observed in 2 experimental situations thought to differentially elicit fear and embarrassment behaviors. In Study 2, 44 children (aged 22 months) were seen in the situations of Study 1 and 3 additional contexts thought to elicit embarrassment behavior. The results of both studies indicate that embarrassme...
408 CitationsSource
This article shows that infants become aware of their competence in the second year. It is argued that this progression is: (1) promoted in the interaction with a familiar and motivated adult; (2) is part and parcel of developmental advances in control of action; (3) is expressed in a sequence of behavioural indicators predicted by a task-analysis. Task-centred interactions of 12 mother-infant pairs were observed at bimonthly intervals between the infants' 14th and 22nd month. Control of action ...
26 CitationsSource
#1Heidelise Als (Boston Children's Hospital)H-Index: 38
#2Frank H. Duffy (Boston Children's Hospital)H-Index: 49
Last. Gloria B. McAnulty (Boston Children's Hospital)H-Index: 29
view all 3 authors...
Abstract This study investigates the association between gestational age at birth and behavioral functioning 2 weeks after due date. Ninety-eight healthy newborns were stratified into three gestational age groups—early-born preterms (PPT), 27 to 32 weeks; middle-group preterms (PT), 33 to 37 weeks; and full-terms (FT), 38 to 41 weeks—and their behavior was examined using the system scores of the Assessment of Preterm Infants' Behavior (APIB). All infants were free of medical complications. The P...
68 CitationsSource
#1Carol S. DweckH-Index: 83
#2Ellen L. Leggett (Harvard University)H-Index: 2
Past work has documented and described major patterns of adaptive and maladaptive behavior: the mastery-oriented and the helpless patterns. In this article, we present a research-based model that accounts for these patterns in terms of underlying psychological processes. The model specifies how individuals' implicit theories orient them toward particular goals and how these goals set up the different patterns. Indeed, we show how each feature (cognitive, affective, and behavioral) of the adaptiv...
5,682 CitationsSource
#1H Blocklewis (Yale University)H-Index: 7
Let me first express my delight at the chance to discuss the psychoanalysis of shame and guilt with neuro-behavioral scientists. I have been devoting the past 25 years of my life to these two grisly states — in my view human beings’ principal moral emotions. And of course it does not take much thinking to realize that the reason I have had to focus on shame and guilt is that in psychoanalytic work and psychotherapeutic work of any kind these are the two prevailing emotions with which clients add...
221 CitationsSource
#1Heinz Heckhausen (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 24
14 Citations
#1Heinz Heckhausen (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 24
20 Citations
Cited By184
Newest
#1David J. K. Hardecker (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 1
#2Daniel B. M. Haun (MPG: Max Planck Society)H-Index: 28
Abstract When do hurt feelings develop? In this paper, we want to set the stage for empirical investigations that can answer this neglected question. Thus, we present an integrative theory of hurt feelings according to which hurt feelings in their narrow sense consist of (a) a primary appraisal of an illegitimate devaluation, (b) a secondary appraisal of low controllability, (c) an action tendency to withdraw, and (d) the communicative function to signal that one has been wronged, and is expecti...
Source
#1Zachary Witkower (UBC: University of British Columbia)H-Index: 4
#2Eric Mercadante (UBC: University of British Columbia)H-Index: 1
Last. Jessica L. Tracy (UBC: University of British Columbia)H-Index: 38
view all 3 authors...
All human societies are organized hierarchically, and individuals who occupy positions of high social rank typically acquire fitness advantages over lower ranking group members. Here, we argue that certain emotions function, at least in part, to help individuals successfully navigate these hierarchies. We review evidence suggesting that nine distinct emotions – pride, shame, anger, fear, sadness, disgust, contempt, envy, and admiration – influence social rank outcomes in important ways; most not...
Source
#1Eglantine Julle-Danière (University of Portsmouth)H-Index: 3
#2Jamie Whitehouse (University of Portsmouth)H-Index: 7
Last. Bridget M. Waller (University of Portsmouth)H-Index: 28
view all 7 authors...
Guilt is a complex emotion with a potentially important social function of stimulating cooperative behaviours towards and from others, but whether the feeling of guilt is associated with a recognisable pattern of nonverbal behaviour is unknown. We examined the production and perception of guilt in two different studies, with a total of 238 participants with various places of origin. Guilt was induced experimentally, eliciting patterns of movement that were associated with both the participants’ ...
Source
#2Eva PilaH-Index: 1
Last. Jenna D. GilchristH-Index: 5
view all 3 authors...
1 CitationsSource
#1Susana Conejero (UPV/EHU: University of the Basque Country)H-Index: 4
#2Pedro Apodaca (UPV/EHU: University of the Basque Country)H-Index: 7
Last. María-José Ortiz (UPV/EHU: University of the Basque Country)H-Index: 3
view all 5 authors...
AbstractThis study analysed the influence of praise and criticism on moral pride (the pride we feel when we believe we have done something morally good) and guilt. Ninety-two adolescents (age: M = ...
1 CitationsSource
#1Daniel Sznycer (UdeM: Université de Montréal)H-Index: 14
#2Aaron W. Lukaszewski (CSUF: California State University, Fullerton)H-Index: 13
Abstract Social emotions are hypothesized to be adaptations designed by selection to solve adaptive problems pertaining to social valuation—the disposition to attend to, associate with, and aid a target individual based on her probable contributions to the fitness of the valuer. To steer between effectiveness and economy, social emotions need to activate in precise proportion to the local evaluations of the various acts and characteristics that dictate the social value of self and others. Suppor...
2 CitationsSource
#1Stefania SetteH-Index: 15
#2Danielle Baldwin (Carleton University)H-Index: 4
Last. Robert J. Coplan (Carleton University)H-Index: 38
view all 5 authors...
In early education contexts, shy children are prone to experiencing difficulties in their relationships with peer and teachers. These negative social experiences may, in turn, reinforce shy children's feelings of self-consciousness. The aim of the present study was to test a complex model linking shyness with self-conscious emotions (i.e., guilt, shame) through negative social experiences at preschool (i.e., peer difficulties, non-supportive teacher–child relationships). Participants were 131 (6...
Source
#1Uta K. Bindl (LSE: London School of Economics and Political Science)H-Index: 7
Organizations benefit from proactive employees who initiate improvements at work. Although evidence suggests happy employees are more likely to become proactive, the emotional journeys employees take during the process of making things happen, and their implications for future proactivity at work, remain unclear. To develop an understanding of patterns of emotions in the process of proactivity, I conducted a qualitative study based on 92 proactivity episodes by employees and their managers in th...
1 CitationsSource
#1Daniel Sznycer (UdeM: Université de Montréal)H-Index: 14
Pride, shame, and guilt color our highest and lowest personal moments. Recent evidence suggests that these self-conscious emotions are neurocognitive adaptations crafted by natural selection. Specifically, self-conscious emotions solve adaptive problems of social valuation by promoting the achievement of valued actions and characteristics to increase others’ valuations of the individual (pride); limiting information-triggered devaluation (shame); and remedying events where one put insufficient w...
4 CitationsSource
#1Michael Lewis (RU: Rutgers University)H-Index: 81
The self-conscious emotions according to the theory presented here require two important features which develop in the first 3 years of life. These are consciousness, defined here as self-awareness as measured by self-recognition in mirrors, the onset of personal pronouns like “me” or “mine,” and complex pretend play. These emerge between 15 and 24 months of age and give rise to self-conscious-exposed emotions such as embarrassment, envy, and empathy, as well as prosocial behaviors such as shari...
1 CitationsSource